Absolute Identification Processes Research Paper

Exclusively available on Available only on IvyPanda® Made by Human No AI

It is interesting that there is a finding in cognitive research on absolute identification (AI) that participants can normally discriminate between stimuli that are different (such as red and black size 8 shoes), but there is a greater error when trying to differentiate between the same stimuli. With regard to repeatedly choosing sizes 7 and 9 when looking for an 8, even though 8 is a very popular size in women’s shoes, it may be because there is only minimal difference between a 7 to an 8, or an 8 to a 9. Arrangement of the shoes in numerical size order would increase the likelihood of such a mistake because the differences between the sizes of the same type of shoe would be minimal and not stand out. Absolute identification processes mean that a person learns unique associations between stimuli that are variable on a simple perceptual level, such as color, texture, smell, taste, or shape. Central stimuli, in this case, size 8, and its neighbors sizes 7 and 9, and stimuli in large sets (such as a style lined up from smallest to largest size) have been found to be responded to more slowly and significantly less accurate. In contrast, when stimuli are widely spaced, there is a tendency for a person to respond slightly more accurately (as when shoes are displayed not in rows but on different display heights and distances from each other), although widely spaced stimuli tend to also be responded to slightly more slowly, as compared to stimuli that are narrowly spaced. Evidently, there is a cognitive capacity trade-off between accuracy and speed. In a shoe store, it is likely that the accuracy of obtaining the desired size would be a more important factor. Even though the modern-day maxim tends to be “faster faster” and “I want it now,” if accuracy is low, then the customer will have to wait longer, so accuracy again may tend to be the favored factor when a salesperson is choosing a size wanted by the customer.

This limit in the absolute identification of stimuli is supported across a range of sensory stimuli (not just shoes), and it has been concluded this indicates a fundamental property of human cognition.

Again the more similar the stimuli, the more likely it is to appear as simple stimuli, and the central limit will apply. So that shoes that are most different in styles should be responded to quicker and more accurately, and the closer the styles are in similarity, the longer a salesperson’s responsibility to identify them should be, and also less accurate. Also, there are sequential effects at play, meaning that assimilation to recent responses (i.e., the last style identified) and contrast to less recent responses (i.e., the first style identified) will result in slower and less accurate identification in the former, and vice versa in the latter. It appears that the reduction of similarity when mixing styles together enhances the speed and accuracy of absolute identification. Further, individuals tend to have a higher accuracy of absolute identification when there is only a limited number (<7) of distinct unidimensional stimuli (such as shoe styles). It appears to be that as n approaches 7 a person will increase the number of errors they make with absolute identification, as well as responding more slowly. Whereas with comparative judgments, there is much greater differentiation between pairs of a and b. However, across more complex stimuli, absolute identification accuracy has been noted to increase. This finding complements literature about capacity limits when identifying simple (unidimensional) stimuli, although not complex stimuli. There seems to be an opposite type of processing deficit between the identification of simple and complex stimuli so that a person is normally able to process a highly variable set of stimuli more efficiently as a function of the choices available.

It would be possible to train a salesperson to increase accuracy and speed of absolute identification higher than 7 by reorganizing displays and storerooms to take advantage of differences between styles, placing those most similar at greater distances from each other. However, there is a cognitive capacity limit, and it is likely to be rare that a salesperson could achieve a consistently higher than average accuracy and speed rate above 10 styles.

More related papers Related Essay Examples
Cite This paper
You're welcome to use this sample in your assignment. Be sure to cite it correctly

Reference

IvyPanda. (2021, August 29). Absolute Identification Processes. https://ivypanda.com/essays/absolute-identification-processes/

Work Cited

"Absolute Identification Processes." IvyPanda, 29 Aug. 2021, ivypanda.com/essays/absolute-identification-processes/.

References

IvyPanda. (2021) 'Absolute Identification Processes'. 29 August.

References

IvyPanda. 2021. "Absolute Identification Processes." August 29, 2021. https://ivypanda.com/essays/absolute-identification-processes/.

1. IvyPanda. "Absolute Identification Processes." August 29, 2021. https://ivypanda.com/essays/absolute-identification-processes/.


Bibliography


IvyPanda. "Absolute Identification Processes." August 29, 2021. https://ivypanda.com/essays/absolute-identification-processes/.

If, for any reason, you believe that this content should not be published on our website, please request its removal.
Updated:
This academic paper example has been carefully picked, checked and refined by our editorial team.
No AI was involved: only quilified experts contributed.
You are free to use it for the following purposes:
  • To find inspiration for your paper and overcome writer’s block
  • As a source of information (ensure proper referencing)
  • As a template for you assignment
Privacy Settings

IvyPanda uses cookies and similar technologies to enhance your experience, enabling functionalities such as:

  • Basic site functions
  • Ensuring secure, safe transactions
  • Secure account login
  • Remembering account, browser, and regional preferences
  • Remembering privacy and security settings
  • Analyzing site traffic and usage
  • Personalized search, content, and recommendations
  • Displaying relevant, targeted ads on and off IvyPanda

Please refer to IvyPanda's Cookies Policy and Privacy Policy for detailed information.

Required Cookies & Technologies
Always active

Certain technologies we use are essential for critical functions such as security and site integrity, account authentication, security and privacy preferences, internal site usage and maintenance data, and ensuring the site operates correctly for browsing and transactions.

Site Customization

Cookies and similar technologies are used to enhance your experience by:

  • Remembering general and regional preferences
  • Personalizing content, search, recommendations, and offers

Some functions, such as personalized recommendations, account preferences, or localization, may not work correctly without these technologies. For more details, please refer to IvyPanda's Cookies Policy.

Personalized Advertising

To enable personalized advertising (such as interest-based ads), we may share your data with our marketing and advertising partners using cookies and other technologies. These partners may have their own information collected about you. Turning off the personalized advertising setting won't stop you from seeing IvyPanda ads, but it may make the ads you see less relevant or more repetitive.

Personalized advertising may be considered a "sale" or "sharing" of the information under California and other state privacy laws, and you may have the right to opt out. Turning off personalized advertising allows you to exercise your right to opt out. Learn more in IvyPanda's Cookies Policy and Privacy Policy.

1 / 1