Religion and Morality Relation Essay (Critical Writing)

Exclusively available on Available only on IvyPanda® Made by Human No AI

Introduction

Whether religion provides a moral basis is both topical and old. Socrates inquired whether divine beings adore goodness since it is great, or whether honesty is great since the gods love it. Although there is no deficiency of spirited questioning, logical examinations of religion and morality have delivered mixed outcomes. The interpretive challenges are aggravated by loose originations of both religion and morality. This paper seeks to clarify the confusion concerning religion and morality.

The Concept of Religion and Morality

Most people share Laura Schlessinger’s conviction that morality is inconceivable without confidence in God, and in most countries, this mentality is undeniably pervasive (Baumard & Boyer, 2013). In a progression of convincing examinations, some researchers have exhibited a verifiable relationship of nonbelievers with immorality. Although these affiliations are more grounded in individuals who believe in the Supreme Being, freethinkers instinctively see acts, for example, manslaughter and incest as progressively illustrative of cynics than of different religious, ethnic, or social gatherings. Atheists unequivocally repudiate this association, with some recommending that freethinkers are the ethical spine of the society, taking their obligations seriously because they do not confide in God to help humanity from its imprudence (Baumard & Boyer, 2013).

As history can be composed at any amplification, religion, and morality can be investigated at any level of classification. At the limits, one can treat religion and morality as individual elements and try to portray their relationship, or one can contemplate the impact of a specific philosophical precept (destiny) on some profoundly moral result (tithing). The test is to embrace a pragmatic and hypothetically measure of analysis.

Analysts in Neuropsychiatry see mental disorders as unexpected groupings of manifestations and contend that the unit of examination should be the manifestation (hallucinations) as opposed to the disease (Baumard & Boyer, 2013). In like manner, analyzing the relationship may require categorizing these ideas into essential units. Although morality and religion contain autonomous instruments that are associated with stories, teachings, and other appropriated systems of thoughts, each outcome has particular psychological procedures and capacities (Baumard & Boyer, 2013).

As the heavenly bodies of the crystal gazers, these gatherings of psychological and social characteristics and propensities might be fake, unforeseen, and self-assertive, rather than grounded in any stable fundamental uniformity (Kelemen, Rottman, & Seston, 2013). One remarkable element of Saroglou’s model of religious measurements is that it classifies morality as a critical component of religion. This provision suggests that any investigation into the impacts of religion on morality might be a futile task.

Descriptive Ethnocentrism

One impediment to the analysis of religion and morality is the propensity of analysts to use their social perspective in describing a moral concern. Whatever the ethical assessment, a multifaceted investigation between religion and morality must expand the moral sphere beyond the beliefs of people in Western, developed, opulent, and law-based social orders (Friese & Wänke, 2014). Religion and morality are discretionary and do not suggest flexible common structures, endeavors to connect them. Thus, moral conduct is not a behavior we advocate; it is the desired conduct on ethical standards.

Religion and Morality

An extensive clarification in developmental terms of any causal connections between the fractionated parts of religion and morality would consider four kinds of inquiries, known as Tinbergen’s Four Whys:

  1. A causal why concerning the psychological systems that deliver a specific connection between religion and morality.
  2. A formative why concerning the procedures by which the relationship develops in the evolution of individuals.
  3. A utilitarian why relating to the versatile value of the relationship with others.
  4. A functional why concerning the theory of association and its appearance through structures.

Fractionating Morality

Moral scholars suggest that societal norms are composed of the human instinct, emerging from the ordinary activity of developed subjective components. Ethical establishments are imagined as obliging, rather than deciding, the kinds of good frameworks that people build (Friese & Wänke, 2014). One of the real commitments of the ethical paradigms has been to feature the social and political inconsistency in the outflow of these foundations.

A few societies build their ethical standards and organizations on a relatively little subset of these foundations. For instance, while the ethical request of most conventional social orders is expansive, the moral paradigm is contrived on “individualizing” establishments, concentrating on protecting people from hurt and abuse (Friese & Wänke, 2014). Different examinations have discovered that political nonconformists value the individualizing standards of consideration and fairness more than traditionalists do, though traditionalists value the official rules of faithfulness and holiness more than liberals (Friese & Wänke, 2014)

Fractionating Religion

As it is conceivable to categorize morality into layers of hypothetical components, religion can be fractionated into segments with solid subjective underpinnings. Works of literature in the ‘intellectual study of religion’ have not revealed the comprehensiveness of specific religious interpretations, for example, different ideas of ancestors, gods, or penances, favors, and ritual experiences. Religious analysts suggest the assortment of socially circulated beliefs that have been marked religion are formed and guarded by a finite and dissimilar set of advanced psychological inclinations called religious establishment.

The Religion and Morality Relationship

The difficulties in understanding why religion changes as social orders become complex identify with evolving structures and capacity of custom. With upward social convulsion, religious traditions have become routinized, principle and account have become standardized, convictions have been universalistic, religion has been classified, organizations more professionalized, and religious writings have been systematized (Laurin & Plaks, 2014).

Many studies use parochial originations of religion and morality but do not categorize these classifications into hypothetically components, or disregard to evaluate the unpredictable interchange between intuition and culture. The propensity to embrace purified social conduct has hampered endeavors to analyze speculations of the uncommon social predominance of admonishing god ideas (Laurin & Plaks, 2014). By implication, people accept practices that enable religious gatherings to endure and expand.

Conclusion

The connection between religion and morality is a profound and emotive theme. The declarations of experts give a false representation of the confounding hypothetical and methodological unpredictability of the issue. In the academic circle, evolution is often blocked by a progression of applied constraints and lacunae. Thus, social representations, ideas, beliefs, antiquities, and practices both endorsed and prohibited are activated, molded, and obliged by an assortment of primary subjective frameworks.

There is no connection between religion and morality. By implication, religion does not provide a moral basis for individuals. Religion and morality are discretionary and do not suggest flexible common structures, endeavors to connect them. Religion and morality, which are considered solid substances, are bound to be superficial or circular. Consequently, a few parts of religion may advance a few perspectives of morality, similarly as others smother or impede the equivalent, or unique, perspectives.

References

Baumard, N., & Boyer, P. (2013). Explaining moral religions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 272-280. Web.

Friese, M., & Wänke, M. (2014). Personal prayer buffers self-control depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 51, 56-59. Web.

Kelemen, D., Rottman, J., & Seston, R. (2013). Professional physical scientists display tenacious teleological tendencies: Purpose-based reasoning as a cognitive default. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 1074-1083. Web.

Laurin, K., & Plaks, E. (2014). Religion and punishment: Opposing influences of orthopraxy and orthodoxy on reactions to unintentional acts. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 5, 835–843. Web.

More related papers Related Essay Examples
Cite This paper
You're welcome to use this sample in your assignment. Be sure to cite it correctly

Reference

IvyPanda. (2021, June 16). Religion and Morality Relation. https://ivypanda.com/essays/religion-and-punishment/

Work Cited

"Religion and Morality Relation." IvyPanda, 16 June 2021, ivypanda.com/essays/religion-and-punishment/.

References

IvyPanda. (2021) 'Religion and Morality Relation'. 16 June.

References

IvyPanda. 2021. "Religion and Morality Relation." June 16, 2021. https://ivypanda.com/essays/religion-and-punishment/.

1. IvyPanda. "Religion and Morality Relation." June 16, 2021. https://ivypanda.com/essays/religion-and-punishment/.


Bibliography


IvyPanda. "Religion and Morality Relation." June 16, 2021. https://ivypanda.com/essays/religion-and-punishment/.

If, for any reason, you believe that this content should not be published on our website, please request its removal.
Updated:
This academic paper example has been carefully picked, checked and refined by our editorial team.
No AI was involved: only quilified experts contributed.
You are free to use it for the following purposes:
  • To find inspiration for your paper and overcome writer’s block
  • As a source of information (ensure proper referencing)
  • As a template for you assignment
1 / 1