Dictator Game Experiment and Behavioral Economics Research Paper

Exclusively available on Available only on IvyPanda®
Updated:
This academic paper example has been carefully picked, checked and refined by our editorial team.
You are free to use it for the following purposes:
  • To find inspiration for your paper and overcome writer’s block
  • As a source of information (ensure proper referencing)
  • As a template for you assignment

Introduction

One of the classical assumptions of behavioral economics is that people act with self-interest in mind. In situations related to financial well-being, personal interests and desires will inherently come first. This is an evolutionary survival technique that applies to modern society as well.

Although the concept of altruism is promoted as a social and behavioral trait, it assumes that a rational human always makes decisions to maximize benefits to themselves in one way or another. Therefore, in economic theory, it is essential to evaluate the rationale and motive of financial behavior in order to determine whether assumptions are correlated with reality within an experimental setting. This paper presents an experiment based on the “dictator game” that investigates the behavior of individuals presented with an opportunity to choose between self-interest and altruism.

Literature Review

The dictator game is a type of bargaining game that attempts to manipulate strategic behavior. It is meant to study the impact of fairness, trust, or altruism in economic decision-making. One of the players selected to be the “dictator” selects the optimal strategy depending on the move of another player and in turn putting pressure on them (“Dictator Game”).

Distribution of Mean Giving per Treatment
Fig. 1: Distribution of Mean Giving per Treatment.

Experimental economics has focused on exploring social behavior. A dictator game makes it possible to reveal potential social motives that drive decision-making in situations of financial inequality and authority. A variety of theoretical models exist to support ideological predispositions for human behavior. Fairness and equity models argue that people are interested in more than absolute self-interest but also relative payoffs as well. Another model suggests that payoffs are based on social preference and efficiency. It has been suggested that reciprocity is a factor which can drive a player’s intention. A popular model focuses on altruism by establishing utility functions for distribution of wealth. However, all models assume that the dynamic of social behavior remains stable and identical over time and repetitions. A dictator game is helpful to observe social behavior with various strategic considerations excluded. It is effective since payoff outcomes can be determined without calculation and there is no interdependence of decisions (Brosig-Koch et al. 2).

A meta-analysis of studies conducting dictator games shows that on average, 28.35% of the endowment is given away by the dictator. A random-effects meta-analysis shows equivalent results. Figure 1 above shows the distribution of the results out of 616 treatments, with the distribution being skewed to the left. In 6 of these, the average remained zero. The distribution of the contributions is uneven, with 36.11% keeping everything, 16.74% splitting evenly, and 5.44% giving away the whole amount. This disproves the null hypothesis observed in most studies and based on the rational choice theory that the give rate will be 0 (Engel 7). It was found that specific manipulating factors had profound effects on the rate of distribution. These include incentives, social control, and social distance. For example, revealed identity or use of real money showed unique effects. Furthermore, demographic data such as age, gender, and even country of birth led to specific patterns of behavior (Engel 25).

Experiment Design and Procedures

Design

Basic concept of a simple dictator game
Figure 2: Basic concept of a simple dictator game.

The diagram above illustrates the basic design concept of the dictator game. The player chosen to be the dictator is provided with a set endowment that must be allocated amongst the players. The giver must offer a set amount to the other player, even if it is equal to 0. The dictator’s decision is final. Meanwhile, the other player has no impact on the strategic outcome. Therefore, the experiment is meant to isolate a subject’s decision under the condition of no reciprocal consideration from the opposing player. “A typical distribution of offers with an initial endowment, e, is bimodal at the predicted offer (e, 0) and the equitable offer (0.5e, 0.5e), where the first element indicates the dictator’s payoff and the second the receiver’s” (Schurter and Wilson 130).

There are a number of influencing factors that can impact the decision made by the dictator. Certain barriers can be established by the experimenter such as a trivia quiz to determine the dictator (earning authority) or changing endowment size in different trials. However, the most significant factor is social distance. The condition of anonymity or communication amongst the players could impact the decision since the dictator would feel social judgment and concerns of fairness if identities were to be revealed (Schurter and Wilson 130). This experiment would attempt to eliminate any potentially influential social factors in order to isolate the specific economic behavior of individuals.

Hypothesis

Under the conditions of anonymity and lack of influencing social factors, subjects selected as dictator will choose to offer ~20% of the money to the other player in the majority of the trials.

Procedures

The experiment will be conducted in a class. It does not require prerequisite knowledge or preparation from the subjects. The experiment should take approximately 15 minutes. Each student is assigned a number which is then randomized by a computer program to determine dictators and receivers. This ensures anonymity since students will be familiar with each other. To encourage truthful participation, it is announced that several random participants will receive monetary payouts.

Dictators are provided with a form (either paper or computerized) that allows them to record their decision of how much to offer the other player out of a $10 value. The forms are collected and mixed. They are then given to the receivers, randomly matched by number. The recipients cannot accept or reject the offer, but must simply fill out how much they have been offered. To collect qualitative data, it may be helpful to include a section for both the dictator and the recipient to indicate their justifications or feelings based on the values. The students then switch roles and repeat the procedure for a second trial.

Interpretation

It is both difficult and inaccurate to generalize experimental results as applicable in a real-world setting. Theoretical frameworks have to account for psychological and economic realities, and the settings of interpersonal and financial interaction. However, the dictator game creates a situation similar to that of a credible, authoritative figure inquiring whether to share money with their less fortunate dependents. In a real-world setting, it is common to find this type of pairing, which makes certain people’s well-being dependent on others, either professionally or politically. There are specific socio-cultural norms that impact human behavior in these cases. However, interpreting experimental results with reference to real-life settings should be done with caution since artificial conditions impose limits and do not account for true moral pressures that could influence behavior. One beneficial use of a simple dictator game is to determine how economic behavior is elicited by institutionalized manipulation (List 491). This aspect is highly applicable to corporations and governments in a real-world setting.

Works Cited

Brosig-Koch, Jeannette, et al. “.” PLoS ONE, vol. 12, no. 4, 2017, pp. 1-18. Web.

.” EconPort, n.d. Web.

Engel, Christoph. . 2010. Web.

List, John A. “.” Journal of Political Economy, vol. 115, no. 3, 2007, pp. 482-493. Web.

Schurter, Karl, and Bart Wilson. “.” Southern Economic Journal, vol. 76, no. 1, 2009, pp. 130-145. Web.

More related papers Related Essay Examples
Cite This paper
You're welcome to use this sample in your assignment. Be sure to cite it correctly

Reference

IvyPanda. (2020, September 26). Dictator Game Experiment and Behavioral Economics. https://ivypanda.com/essays/dictator-game-experiment-and-behavioral-economics/

Work Cited

"Dictator Game Experiment and Behavioral Economics." IvyPanda, 26 Sept. 2020, ivypanda.com/essays/dictator-game-experiment-and-behavioral-economics/.

References

IvyPanda. (2020) 'Dictator Game Experiment and Behavioral Economics'. 26 September.

References

IvyPanda. 2020. "Dictator Game Experiment and Behavioral Economics." September 26, 2020. https://ivypanda.com/essays/dictator-game-experiment-and-behavioral-economics/.

1. IvyPanda. "Dictator Game Experiment and Behavioral Economics." September 26, 2020. https://ivypanda.com/essays/dictator-game-experiment-and-behavioral-economics/.


Bibliography


IvyPanda. "Dictator Game Experiment and Behavioral Economics." September 26, 2020. https://ivypanda.com/essays/dictator-game-experiment-and-behavioral-economics/.

If, for any reason, you believe that this content should not be published on our website, please request its removal.
Updated:
Privacy Settings

IvyPanda uses cookies and similar technologies to enhance your experience, enabling functionalities such as:

  • Basic site functions
  • Ensuring secure, safe transactions
  • Secure account login
  • Remembering account, browser, and regional preferences
  • Remembering privacy and security settings
  • Analyzing site traffic and usage
  • Personalized search, content, and recommendations
  • Displaying relevant, targeted ads on and off IvyPanda

Please refer to IvyPanda's Cookies Policy and Privacy Policy for detailed information.

Required Cookies & Technologies
Always active

Certain technologies we use are essential for critical functions such as security and site integrity, account authentication, security and privacy preferences, internal site usage and maintenance data, and ensuring the site operates correctly for browsing and transactions.

Site Customization

Cookies and similar technologies are used to enhance your experience by:

  • Remembering general and regional preferences
  • Personalizing content, search, recommendations, and offers

Some functions, such as personalized recommendations, account preferences, or localization, may not work correctly without these technologies. For more details, please refer to IvyPanda's Cookies Policy.

Personalized Advertising

To enable personalized advertising (such as interest-based ads), we may share your data with our marketing and advertising partners using cookies and other technologies. These partners may have their own information collected about you. Turning off the personalized advertising setting won't stop you from seeing IvyPanda ads, but it may make the ads you see less relevant or more repetitive.

Personalized advertising may be considered a "sale" or "sharing" of the information under California and other state privacy laws, and you may have the right to opt out. Turning off personalized advertising allows you to exercise your right to opt out. Learn more in IvyPanda's Cookies Policy and Privacy Policy.

1 / 1